INTRODUCTION
As
reflected in many chapters in this book, English is probably the most well studied
language in the history of linguistics, so that there is a vast pool of examples
of both excellent description and insightful theoretical analysis to be found
in the literature. Still, concepts like ‘description’ and ‘theory’ are anything
but clear. The issue of what the defining characteristics of a ‘theory’ are has
received a lot of attention in philosophy and the history of science. However,
in terms of distinguishing a theory from a description, that literature is not
terribly helpful. Even though ‘theory’ may appear to be the more complex of the
two notions, there are issues also with what constitutes a description of a
language.
A description of any language should contain an inventory of
the building blocks; sounds and morphemes, roughly. It should also contain the
rules for how those elements can be combined; phonotactic constraints,
information about which differences between sounds are distinctive, how
morphemes can be combined to form words, and how words can be combined to form
phrases. In spite of the attention that the language has received, no complete
description of English in this sense has yet been provided. To take but one
example, even though there are many insightful descriptions of the English
passive, the exact rules that allow for sentences such as This road has been walked on have not been provided. The view of a
grammatical description just described coincides with the original conception
of a ‘generative’ grammar. A generative grammar in that sense takes the
building blocks of a language and ‘generates’ all and only the grammatical
sentences of that language. Needless to say, no complete such grammar has been
defined, not for English and not for any other language.
No comments:
Post a Comment